After reading the articles assigned this week, I remembered a relavent article I came across for my research about peer revisions in ESL writing. Cassia and Johnson (1994) looked at peer revisions, a method argued by many of their sources to be beneficial for L1 students, and investigated it’s usefulness in L2 writing classes. Within a class of 12 non-native English speakers, students were broken into pairs and asked to give feedback and their peer’s paper, from comprehension to grammar. The researchers then looked at the final drafts of the students to see how often the feedback was used. Finally, the researchers conducted post-interviews to ask the students not only how beneficial the peer reviews were to them personally.
The results of the study showed that 53% of the time students incorporated their peer’s comments. Ten percent of the time, students did not revise a given part of their texts even though it had been discussed in the peer review. Interestingly, 37% of the time, students revised parts of their essays that had not been discussed in the peer reviews, indicating the students picked up other techniques for writing that they saw in other students’ papers. =All but two of the students reported that peer reviews were helpful, and those two students said that it was a “boring” assignment.
Cassia and Johnson conclude that peer feedback is effective because it allows for students to actively analyze and learn their mistakes and other students’ mistakes, as opposed to being passively told by their teachers which revisions to make.
I thought it was interesting that none of the authors from this week’s readings cited this study. The research contributes valuable data to the field of research on grammatical corrective feedback in that it is one of the few that indicate success, and it approaches corrective feedback with a new method - - peer review. This unique approach responds to Ferris’ call for researchers to take grammatical corrective feedback research a step further (2004).
While I believe this research is valuable, Truscott would likely argue that even though the students found the corrective feedback helpful from their peers, students may not be the best judges of what learning style is best for them, thus the study does not sufficiently promote the use corrective feedback. I however, would disagree. While post-interviews may seem skewed because of the self-reporting of students, the success of the peer review was also indicated by the amount of students who chose, with their own free will, to incorporate their peer’s suggestions and many times take a step further to correct other things they have learned from talking with their peers about corrections. This shows that the students not only learned but also applied the corrections.
Cassia, O. M. and Johnson, K. E. (1994). Peer review negotiations: Revision activities in ESL writing instruction. Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages (TESOL) Quarterly, 28(4), 745-769.
Ferris, D. (2004). The ‘‘grammar correction’’ debate in L2 writing: Where are we, and where do we go from here? (and what do we do in the meantime . . .?) . Journal of Second Language Writing, 13, p. 49-62.
Truscott, J. (1996). The case against grammar correction in L2 writing classes. Language Learning, 46, 327-369.
Monday, March 15, 2010
Subscribe to:
Post Comments (Atom)
No comments:
Post a Comment